Tracking (dog)






Male Weimeraner Following a Scent Trail in the Snow


Tracking is a technique in which dogs are trained to locate things while in use by humans by using the object's scent, for a variety of purposes.
Tracking has always been an essential skill for dogs to survive in the wild, through hunting and tracking down potential prey.[1]




Contents






  • 1 Physiological Mechanisms of Tracking


  • 2 Phases of tracking


  • 3 Factors influencing a dog’s tracking ability


  • 4 Uses of Tracking


  • 5 See also


  • 6 References





Physiological Mechanisms of Tracking


Primarily, dogs use their sense of smell, to find and follow a track.[2] Dogs have a highly sensitive olfactory system superior to humans,[3] and are able to discriminate between different humans' scents.[4] Moreover, dogs are also able to use visual cues to follow a track.[5]



Phases of tracking


There are three phases,[1] which complete the process of tracking:


1. Searching Phase


This is the phase in which dogs attempt to find a track. They move quickly, and take short, quick sniffs.

2. Deciding Phase


Once the dog has found the track, they move more slowly and take longer sniffs to determine the direction of the track. This is usually the most difficult phase for the dog.

3. Tracking phase


After determining the direction, dogs follow the track by sniffing at a uniform pace, and moving at a speed similar to that of the searching.


Factors influencing a dog’s tracking ability


There are several factors which influences a dog’s ability to track:
Physiological features of the dog are a factor, including age and sex. As a dog ages, their olfactory acuity decreases and decreases their ability to track.[2] Furthermore, male dogs have been found to be better at tracking than females, possibly due to sex differences in olfaction.[2] Sniffing behaviour also influences olfaction, and therefore a dog’s ability to track. For example, a dog’s ability to sniff is higher when it is not panting due to fatigue – it is physically impossible for a dog to both pant and sniff at the same time and the mouth must be open or closed respectively.[6] However, dogs trained to track during physically demanding activities may have adapted their behaviour by increasing sniffing frequency to maximize olfaction, and tracking.[6] Also, different dog breeds have varying suitability to different tasks of tracking.[3] For example, the Bureau of Alcohol, Tobacco, Firearms and Explosives use Labrador Retrievers for their tracking purposes.


Besides the dog itself, there are other factors influencing tracking ability. A track’s age[5] and cross-contamination of scents,[7] the range of the dog from the trail,[3] as well as the handler’s relationship with the tracking dog[7] can all affect a dog’s tracking ability.



Uses of Tracking


There are several uses for tracking. Several examples include:



  • Tracking in police work as Police dogs. Tracking tasks may include tracking down alive[7] and dead individuals,[3] recovering evidence from a crime scene,[8] finding explosives,[7] drugs,[7] and in arson investigations.[9]

  • Tracking for search and rescuing as search and rescue dogs. Tracking dogs are used to find individuals trapped in avalanches or disaster zones.[3]

  • Tracking in hunting to recover animals shot down by hunters.[5]

  • Tracking as a dog sport, such as in tracking trials using scent trails.



See also


  • Tracking trial


References





  1. ^ ab Thesen, A., Steen, J. B., & Døving, K. B. (1993). Behaviour of dogs during olfactory tracking. Journal of Experimental Biology, 180, 247-251.


  2. ^ abc Wells, D. L., & Hepper, P. G. (2003). Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 84(4), 297-305.


  3. ^ abcde Weakley-Jones, B., & Rebmann, A. J. (2005). DOGS, USE IN POLICE INVESTIGATIONS. In Editor-in-Chief: Jason Payne-James (Ed.), Encyclopedia of forensic and legal medicine (pp. 221-223). Oxford: Elsevier.


  4. ^ Settle, R. H., Sommerville, B. A., McCormick, J., & Broom, D. M. (1994). Human scent matching using specially trained dogs. Animal Behaviour, 48(6), 1443-1448.


  5. ^ abc Steen, J. B., & Wilsson, E. (1990). How do dogs determine the direction of tracks? Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 139, 531-534.


  6. ^ ab Gazit, I., & Terkel, J. (2003). Explosives detection by sniffer dogs following strenuous physical activity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 81(2), 149-161.


  7. ^ abcde Furton, K. G., & Myers, L. J. (2001). The scientific foundation and efficacy of the use of canines as chemical detectors for explosives. Talanta, 54(3), 487-500.


  8. ^ Baldwin, H. B., & Puskarich May, C. (2000). CRIME-SCENE INVESTIGATION AND EXAMINATION | recovery of human remains. In Editor-in-Chief: Jay A. Siegel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of forensic sciences (pp. 447-457). Oxford: Elsevier.


  9. ^ Gialamas, D. M. (1996). Enhancement of fire scene investigations using accelerant detection canines. Science & Justice, 36(1), 51-54.










Popular posts from this blog

Xamarin.iOS Cant Deploy on Iphone

Glorious Revolution

Dulmage-Mendelsohn matrix decomposition in Python